Psychotic Experiences Questionnaire. Part 1

Cover Page


Cite item

Full Text

Open Access Open Access
Restricted Access Access granted
Restricted Access Subscription or Fee Access

Abstract

BACKGROUND: The rationale for the concept of psychotic and psychotic-like experiences, delusions and auditory hallucinations, on the basis of which the Psychotic Experiences Questionnaire was developed.

AIM: The purpose of this study is to present the results of testing and validating of a questionnaire that assesses the severity of psychotic experiences. The study aims to determine the content of the experience at different phases of manifestation, including the phase of psychotic-like experiences, as well as metacognitive appraisals of the experience and its relationship to social anxiety.

MATERIAL AND METHODS: The sample of 122 respondents includes non-clinical and two clinical groups — with symptoms of the affective spectrum and with symptoms of the psychotic spectrum. To assess the convergent validity of the Questionnaire, the Russian-language Symptom Check List-90-Revised was used. Metacognitive appraisals of psychotic experience and the overall severity of social anxiety (“Social Anxiety and Social Anxiety Disorder Questionnaire”) were measured.

RESULTS: The structure of the Questionnaire obtained by cluster analysis was verified by confirmatory factor analysis, and optimal agreement between theoretical and empirical models was demonstrated (CFI=0.998; TLI=0.998; SRMR=0.102; RMSEA=0.02; RMSEA p-value=0.986). Three scales and corresponding subscales (six categories) defining the phases of unfolding of psychotic experience were identified. The high reliability and item consistency of the Questionnaire was determined using Cronbach's alpha coefficient. Convergent validity was assessed using Pearson correlation analysis of the questionnaire scales with the SCL scales — psychoticism, paranoid ideation and obsessive-compulsive symptoms; reliable and adequate statistical relationships were obtained. The role of social anxiety and metacognitive appraisals of psychotic experiences in their actualisation and maintenance is shown. The conceptualisation of thought the emergence of intrusions, “voices” as a dialogical embodiment of the internalised experience of social defeat in re-expanded inner speech is presented.

CONCLUSION: The results of approbation and validation of the questionnaire are presented, and the relationships of the psychotic experiences component with its metacognitive appraisals and social anxiety are described. The concept of psychotic experiences is considered a continuum in which disruptions in the usual controllability of mental processes vary range from mild (objectification of thinking) — to moderate (disturbance of self-perception, intrusive phenomena, including “voices”) — to severe (disturbance of the sense of agency, including command “voices”).

Full Text

BACKGROUND

In traditional psychiatry, hallucinations and delusions were once regarded as primary symptoms, indicating schizophrenia. However, modern scientific understanding suggests that these experiences are not exclusive to psychotic spectrum disorders [1] but also manifest in affective disorders, Alzheimer’s disease, and borderline personality disorder [2]. The lifetime incidence of auditory hallucinations (AH) is 9.6%, with “voices” (verbal AH) occurring in 7.3% of cases [3]. Among adults, the prevalence rates for visual hallucinations are 7.3%, whereas tactile and olfactory hallucinations are reported at 2.6% and 1.5%, respectively [4].

Experiences similar to delusions and hallucinations are prevalent in the general population, with a prevalence of 7.2% and a range of 1.2%–25.5%, among individuals who do not meet the criteria for a mental disorder [5]. Delusion-like experiences have an incidence ranging from 3% to 91% in the global population [6].

In a dimensional framework of psychopathology, as opposed to a categorical one, the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) research methodology has verified a model where hallucinations and delusions represent a continuum of severity and stability, escalating from health to psychotic disorders [5]. Consequently, the conventional view in pathophysiology regarding the unity of mental activity mechanism in normal pathological conditions [7] finds affirmation in current scientific models of mental disorders, providing a reasonable guide based on general psychological patterns for understanding these disorders.

Efforts have been made to consistently operationalize the definition of experiences similar to delusions and/or hallucinations observed in the population. However, proposed terms often have a clinical focus, such as “abnormal”, “prepsychotic”, “subclinical”, “prodromal” experiences [8]. The concept of “Psychotic-Like Experiences (PLEs)” is gradually being established in the scientific thesaurus, contributing to the depathologization of these experiences [9].

PLEs have been documented in children and adolescents, where non-malignant PLEs are typical at this age but can indicate the risk of future Psychotic Experiences (PEs) and comorbid disorders [10, 11]. Distress and dysfunction associated with PLE are key indicators of the need for medical attention [12]. Factors that may distinguish more significant PLEs include the simultaneous presence of internalizing manifestations (such as anxiety disorders and obsessive thoughts) and/or externalizing manifestations (like impulsive behavior and addictions) of disorders. PLEs can also be associated with somatic diseases [13], socio-demographic factors [14], and may be triggered by conditions like sleep deprivation, sensory, and social factors [15].

Current models examining PLE, delirium, and AH discuss the influence of psychological factors on symptom development and maintenance. The evolution of psychological concepts surrounding “voices” is evident in the progression from transforming dysfunctional thoughts about these experiences and their associated maladaptive behavioral responses to analyzing hallucinations as a perceptual-cognitive activity, characterized by a distorted perception of inner speech as a result of a disruption in the sense of agency [16]. This path further leads to viewing hallucinations as a psychosocial phenomenon that embodies the internalized dialogical experience in a system of relationships [17], often associated with negative social emotions [18, 19].

The concept of violation of the sense of agency, which is integral to the RDoC methodology, represents the highest form of self-perception distortion, (perception of oneself as a subject), indicating a loss of authorship over one’s mental processes. This violation manifests as derealization, depersonalization, and uncontrollable intrusions when self-perception disruption does not reach its maximum extent of psychic alienation [20].

A similar concept is present in the works of A.A. Mehrabyan, who emphasizes that both the syndrome of mental automatism and the phenomenon of depersonalization result from a violation of specialized systemic “gnostic feelings”, whose disintegration is evident in cases of psychopathology [21]. In psychological explorations of delusions, significant emphasis is placed on metacognitive factors (such as hasty conclusions, and a predisposition to discount contradicting evidence), the proximity of formation mechanisms, increased Social Anxiety (SA) and paranoia, impaired mental control, and interpersonal events [22].

When studying PLE and PE, it is crucial to consider the degree of alienation of these experiences from habitual experience as a stationary mode of mental activity. Experiments conducted by T.A. Klimusheva under the guidance of B.V. Zeigarnik revealed that error awareness and the complexity of tasks requiring mental exertion create conditions conducive to the manifestation of symptoms of mental automatism (wherein erroneous decision is attributed to “interference” from external sources) [23]. For a psychological understanding of PLE, it is essential to analyze circumstances that intensify alienation and make symptoms of impaired mental agency more conspicuous, often occurring in social interaction situations [24].

The concept has been advanced that PEs are triggered and perpetuated cyclically by traumatic experiences [17], which may include instances of social defeat. It has been proposed to view verbal AHs as dissociative rather than solely as a psychotic one [25]. Delusions, hallucinations, and dissociation appear to exist along a common continuum with normal functioning, constituting transdiagnostic phenomena [26].

In psychology, there is a shortage of modern research, diagnostic tools, and experimental psychological studies of PE and PLE. In broader scientific discourse, a contradiction is observed between the significant interest in understanding PLE, delirium, and AH and the lack of a methodological model for these experiences and their components. Consequently, international researchers are actively developing and implementing methodologies to assess both the quantitative and qualitative attributes of PE and PLE, exemplified by tools such as “Voice and You” [27], the “Questionnaire of Beliefs about Voices” [28], and the “Questionnaire of Tendency to Delusions and Persecutory Ideas” [29], etc. However, existing diagnostic tools mainly focus on recording these phenomena without considering the psychological mechanisms underlying phase transitions characterized by increasing alienation. Addressing the patterns of PE development will expand our understanding of the dynamics of psychotic conditions.

This article outlines the results derived from testing and validating the PE questionnaire, which was designated to assess the intensity of these experiences and determine their substantive characteristics across various phases of manifestation, including the stage of PLE or initial and moderately severe PE. Additionally, the questionnaire assesses the metacognitive aspects associated with these experiences. The objectives of this study include assessing the psychometric properties of this diagnostic tool, statistically verifying the identified structural model of the questionnaire, and interpreting the relationships between the components of PE and SA.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

The development process of the Psychotic Experiences Questionnaire (PEQ) comprised several stages:

  1. During Stage 1, an exhaustive literature review of PLE and PE was conducted. Drawing from key theoretical models, the core manifestations of these experiences, as self-reported by patients with various disorders and conditions, were generated. Expert input from clinical psychologists and psychiatrists specializing in PLE and PE refined the descriptions, yielding 23 questionnaire items. The questionnaire’s instructions were developed with an emphasis on the normativity aspect of PLE under certain conditions (Table 1).

 

Table 1. Psychotic Experiences Questionnaire

Таблица 1. Опросник психотических переживаний

Below is a list of sensations or experiences that individuals sometimes experience. These experiences can occur from time to time and are common during certain states such as insomnia, extreme fatigue, after illness, or during times of stress. Rate the extent to which this statement applies to you.

Points

False

Probably false

Probably true

True

1. People around me can know what I am thinking

    

2. Others can read my mind

    

3. My thoughts are spoken by others

    

4. The results of my activities are perceived as if they were “replaced” (not mine)

    

5. My thoughts seem to be voiced inside my head

    

6. The taste of familiar food is changed

    

7. Unusual images or scenes appear inside my head, as if imposed from outside

    

8. At times, thoughts seem to stop in my head

    

9. I find it difficult to control the way I think

    

10. A voice is sometimes heard inside or near the head

    

11. At times there are so many thoughts in my head that it is difficult to concentrate

    

12. At times, the surroundings are perceived as not quite real, as if I were watching a film

    

13. Someone is controlling my thoughts

    

14. I look at myself as if from the outside or perspective

    

15. I perceive my surroundings as if through a barrier — water or glass

    

16. My movements can be controlled

    

17. My sensations in the body are caused from the outside (they are “made” by someone)

    

18. Voices can make me do things I do not want to do

    

19. What I do ends up being different from what I did

    

20. I am being tested for technologies that influence the psyche

    

21. I can transmit messages to others at a distance with the power of my thoughts

    

22. At times, it is as if certain sounds or scenes are “turning on” in my head

    

23. My emotions and feelings seem not to be mine as if they are changed or imposed by some force from the outside

    

Processing the results involves assessing answers on a 4-point scale: 0 points — false; 1 point — probably false; 2 points — probably true; and 3 points — true.

Scores on scales and subscales are calculated as the sum of scores on items that make up their content.

For Scale 1 “Objectification of thinking and control deficit”, consider items 5, 8, 9, and 11.

For Scale 2 “Impaired self-perception and intrusive phenomena”, calculate the sum of scores of its constituent subscales:

(1) “Derealization, depersonalization” — items 12, 14, and 15.

(2) “Alienation of habitual experience” — items 4, 6, and 19.

(3) “Intrusive experiences inside the head (voices, sounds, scenes)” — items 7, 10, and 22.

For Scale 3 “Violation of psychic agency”, sum the points of the subscales that form it:

(1) “Openness of thoughts” — items 1, 2, 3, and 21.

(2) “Experience of controllability from the outside” — items 13, 16, 17, 18, 20, and 23.

The overall severity of PE is calculated as the sum of points on three scales.

 

Final adjustments were made following tests for question clarity and respondent understanding, ensuring alignment with intended experiences. Discussions with patients undergoing treatment in a psychiatric clinic in a hospital environment, particularly those in partial remission, informed these refinements.

Additionally, PE severity was assessed through metacognitive assessments, covering experiences such as shame, guilt, anxiety, difficulty controlling experiences, and attempts to conceal them. These were rated on a Likert scale from 0 to 3 (No, Rather no, Yes, Rather yes), assessing the role of metacognitive processing in maintaining PE.

To characterize the sample, analyze the role of additional variables, and exclude deviating values, a questionnaire of information regarding the respondent was used. Gender, age, education, and experience of seeking psychological and medical help for PE and PLE were recorded.

  1. Stage 2 comprises studying the cluster structure of the questionnaire and its verification using confirmatory factor analysis, and determining the reliability and internal consistency of the questionnaire items.
  2. In Stage 3, a study of the convergent validity of PEQ was conducted. Thus, we used the “Questionnaire for the Severity of Psychopathological Symptoms” [30], which is an adaptation of the Symptom Check List-90-Revised methodology for a Russian-speaking sample (authored by L.R. Derogatis et al., adapted from N.V. Tarabrin). We focused on the scales of “psychoticism” (10 items), “symptoms of paranoia” (6 items), and “obsessive-compulsive symptoms” (10 items). The severity of symptoms was rated on a Likert scale ranging from 0 (Not at all) to 4 (Very much).
  3. In Stage 4, to determine the role of psychological components in the structure of PE, we not only analyzed metacognitive assessments but also explored the connections of PEQ, its scales, and subscales with SA. Thus, we used the author’s “Questionnaire for SA and SA Disorder” [24], which comprises 29 questions. The questionnaire assesses anxiety levels in various social situations, considering their severity, tendency toward avoidance, and post-event rumination. Responses were rated on a Likert scale ranging from 0 (No) to 3 (Yes).

The study involved 122 respondents aged between 18 and 50 years (Me=26.1 years, SD=8.6 years), comprising 26.2% men and 73.8% women. Participants were categorized based on mental health characteristics into three groups: (1) non-clinical and two clinical groups based on the International Classification of Diseases, 10th revision criteria: (2) those with symptoms of the affective spectrum (57.1% with F41.2 mixed anxiety and depressive disorder; 42.9% with F32.0 depressive episode), and (3) those with psychotic spectrum symptoms (57.1% with F20.0 paranoid schizophrenia; 23.8% with F25.0 schizoaffective disorder; 19.0% with F21.0 schizotypal disorder).

The general criteria for inclusion in the study were age between 18 and 60 years and a minimum of secondary general education. In particular, for inclusion in clinical samples, participants have to be in remission or a state of partial remission where the severity of PLE and/or PE decreases but remains relevant. The non-clinical group consisted of participants without a psychiatric diagnosis.

The exclusion criteria included cognitive-mnestic decline, emotional-volitional defect, and severe mental illness leading to frequent re-hospitalizations. Table 2 presents the characteristics of the groups.

 

Table 2. Group characteristics

Таблица 2. Характеристики групп

Characteristics

Non-clinical group (n=73)

Clinical groups

Group with affective symptoms (n=28)

Group with psychotic symptoms (n=21)

Gender. %

Women

82.2

75.0

42.9

Men

17.8

25.0

57.1

Age. years

Range

18–55

18–38

18–49

25.9

24.5

30.0

SD

8.5

5.6

11.1

Education. %

Higher

34.2

27.6

23.8

Incomplete higher

31.5

23.3

19.7

Secondary vocational

9.6

16.4

20.8

General secondary

17.8

32.7

35.6

Not reported

6.8

 

The study was conducted at Yu.K. Erdman Altai Regional Clinical Psychiatric Hospital, and part of the data (from the non-clinical group) was collected through a random online survey. Participants completed the methods using the PsyToolkit web service [31]. The study was conducted voluntarily and anonymously, and the obtained results were analyzed collectively.

For mathematical and statistical data processing, we used software packages including IBM SPSS 26, JASP 0.16.4, and Jamovi 2.2.3.0. The methods of mathematical and statistical data analysis comprised cluster analysis (using Ward’s method and squared Euclidean distance), confirmatory factor analysis, Cronbach’s alpha coefficient calculation, correlation analysis (Pearson, Spearman, Kendall), one-way analysis of variance, and post hoc test (specifically Fisher’s least significant difference test).

RESULTS AND DISCUSSION

The PEQ scales were analyzed alongside correlations of PE with Symptom Check List (SCL) scales1. By using clustering on the matrix of responses to PEQ items from non-clinical and clinical samples (using Ward’s method and squared Euclidean distance), three scales and corresponding subscales for PLE and PE (totaling six categories) were formed. The approach allowed for the differentiation of items across scales, illustrating the logical grouping of symptoms from mild to moderate (Scales 1 and 2) and severe manifestations of mental alienation (Scale 3).

The assessment of PE yielded six categories: Scale 1: “Objectification of Thinking and Control Deficit” (including “objectification of the thinking process”). Scale 2: “Impaired Self-Perception and Intrusive Phenomena” (comprising subscales such as “derealization and depersonalization”, “alienation of habitual experience”, and “intrusive experiences inside the head” like voices, sounds, scenes)”]. Scale 3: “Violation of Mental Agency” (including subscales like “openness of thoughts” and “experience of controllability from the outside”).

The six categories that form three scales represent the desired empirical structure of the experience of PLE and PE. Confirmatory factor analysis was used to verify the resulting structure of the PEQ, demonstrating optimal consistency between the theoretical and empirical models (CFI=0.998; TLI=0.998; SRMR=0.102; RMSEA=0.02; RMSEA p-value=0.986) (Table 3).

 

Table 3. Results of confirmatory factor analysis

Таблица 3. Результаты конфирматорного факторного анализа

Factors

Factor loadings

I. Objectification of thinking and control deficit

Factor 1. Objectification of the thinking process

5. My thoughts seem to be voiced inside my head

0.478

8. At times, thoughts seem to stop in my head

0.723

9. I find it difficult to control the way I think

0.898

11. At times there are so many thoughts in my head that it is difficult to concentrate

0.762

II. Impaired self-perception and intrusive phenomena

Factor 2. Derealization, depersonalization

12. At times, the surroundings are perceived as not quite real, as if I were watching a film

0.820

14. I look at myself as if from the outside or perspective

0.856

15. I perceive my surroundings as if through a barrier — water or glass

0.914

Factor 3. Alienation of habitual experience

4. The results of my activities are perceived as if they were “replaced” (not mine)

0.815

6. The taste of familiar food is changed

0.674

19. What I do ends up being different from what I did

0.681

Factor 4. Intrusive experiences “inside the head” (voices, sounds, scenes)

7. Unusual images or scenes appear inside my head, as if imposed from outside

0.801

10. A voice is sometimes heard inside or near the head

0.802

22. At times, it is as if certain sounds or scenes are “turning on” in my head

0.724

III. Violation of mental agency

Factor 5. Openness of thoughts

1. People around me can know what I am thinking

0.815

2. Others can read my mind

0.846

3. My thoughts are spoken by others

0.634

21. I can transmit messages to others at a distance with the power of my thoughts

0.721

Factor 6. Experience of controllability from the outside

13. Someone is controlling my thoughts

0.829

16. My movements can be controlled

0.955

17. My sensations in the body are caused from the outside (they are “made” by someone)

0.758

18. Voices can make me do things I do not want to do

0.793

20. I am being tested for technologies that influence the psyche

0.638

23. My emotions and feelings seem not to be mine as if they are changed or imposed by some force from the outside

0.747

Note. All factor loadings in the model are presented in Table 2, at p < 0.001.

Примечание. Все факторные нагрузки в модели, представленной в табл. 2, при р <0,001.

 

The high reliability and consistency of the PEQ scale items were established through Cronbach’s alpha coefficient analysis (Table 4). This internal consistency enables the calculation of scores on scales and facilitates the determination of an integrative indicator of PE severity.

 

Table 4. Cronbach's alpha coefficient of the scales and the total summary score of the Psychotic Experiences Questionnaire

Таблица 4. Коэффициент альфа Кронбаха шкал и общего суммарного показателя опросника психотических переживаний

Coefficient

Scale 1

Scale 2

Scale 3

Total PE indicator

Cronbach’s Alpha

0.724

0.842

0.744

0.890

 

The convergent validity of PEQ was assessed using Pearson’s correlation analysis (r) of its scales with the SCL scales such as “psychoticism”, “paranoia symptoms”, and “obsessive-compulsive symptoms”.

Significant direct relationships were observed between the severity of response on the SCL “psychoticism” scales and all PEQ scales, subscales, and the total PEQ indicator (correlation coefficient r=0.355–0.669; p <0.0001). Specifically, the subscale “experience of controllability from the outside” was associated with “psychoticism” (r=0.379; p <0.0001) and “symptoms of paranoia” (r=0.279; p <0.01), whereas the association with “obsessive-compulsive symptoms” did not reach statistical significance (r=0.168; p=0.065). Notably, the only association that did not reach statistical significance was between obsessive-compulsive symptoms and a violation of mental agency at the height of alienation (“the experience of controllability from the outside”), although the trend toward a direct connection remained evident.

The observed decrease in the clarification of correlation with symptoms indicative of anxiety [18, 19], as alienation intensifies, suggests not the absence of associations but rather the challenge of capturing them through linear psychometric methods (which rely on subjective self-reflection).

The “openness of thoughts” subscale had significant correlations with the SCL scales (“obsessive-compulsive symptoms” with r=0.372, p <0.0001; “symptoms of paranoia” with r=0.380, p <0.0001; “psychoticism” with r=0.355, p <0.0001). The strongest correlations of the SCL scales were observed with subscales of scale 2 (“derealization, depersonalization”; “alienation of habitual experience, intrusive experiences”), Scale 1 (“objectification of thinking”) of the PEQ (r=0.411–0.584; p <0.0001), and its total indicator (r=0.539–0.669; p <0.0001).

Both the PEQ and SCL items assess the experience of “voices”. In the PEQ, the two items specifically measure the experience of AH.

The item “Voices can make me do things I do not want to do” is part of the Scale 3 “violation of mental agency”, in the subscale “experience of controllability from the outside”. This item captures the experience of commanding “voices” that exert “control over a person”, leading to the implementation of unwanted actions, often resulting in confrontation and attempts to suppress such AHs.

This scale represents a pattern of pronounced experience regarding the unfamiliarity of mental phenomena and external influences. It signifies a phase marked by intensified alienation, during which individuals struggle to perceive themselves as agents of their psyche, and the execution of actions loses its inherent volitional nature. This paragraph contains the contrasting concept of “I/mine versus not me/not mine/voice”, attributing this attribution to one’s own experience becomes progressively challenging as alienation increases.

This particular item within the PEQ significantly correlates with a statement from the SCL “psychoticism” scale, specifically “You hear voices that others do not hear” [Spearman’s rho (ρ)=0.382; p <0.0001]. However, despite its high reliability, it demonstrates a lower correlation coefficient than the question about “voices inside the head”.

The item “A voice is sometimes heard inside or near the head” encodes AH with a moderate level of psyche alienation. In this phase, characterized by impaired self-perception, there exists a distorted subject position concerning one’s experiences, indicative of weakened mental control possibilities. This item is part of the “impaired self-perception and intrusive phenomena” scale in the subscale “intrusive experiences inside the head” (voices, sounds, scenes). These intrusions include experiences of mental phenomena that manifest against one’s will, vividly and realistically.

The item under discussion has a significant correlation with the SCL question “You hear voices that others do not hear” (ρ=0.663; p <0.0001), confirming its diagnostic relevance. Unlike SCL, the PEQ lacks the information component “others do not hear”, which requires not only identifying experiences but also comparing them to others’ perceptions. This complicates the item discussed in PEQ, as it is not merely about the dyad “I hear and others do not hear”, which is challenging to differentiate when self-perception is impaired. Instead, it involves contemplating the proximity of the “voice” source localization. The item’s ambiguity regarding localization and periodicity over time provides the necessary spread of data for assessing agreement or disagreement with the item.

The emergence of intrusive, partially alienated mental phenomena “inside the head” (such as “Unusual images or scenes appear inside my head, as if imposed from outside” and “At times, it is as if certain sounds or scenes are ‘turning on’ in my head”) within the subscale significantly correlates with the item about AH in the SCL (ρ=0.367; p <0.0001 and ρ=0.359; p <0.0001, respectively).

The experiences of “voices” were compared between the clinical and non-clinical groups. A one-way analysis of variance revealed a statistically significant difference in the severity of “voices” experience recorded by PEQ items among the non-clinical and two clinical groups. Fisher’s test yielded a value of F(2, 119)=13.10; p <0.0001, for the item regarding the presence of “voice inside or near the head”, and F(2, 119)=3.21; p <0.05 for the item concerning the presence of commanding “voices”.

Post hoc testing using Fisher’s least significant difference test for pairwise mean differences demonstrated that the non-clinical and clinical groups with affective symptoms were statistically indistinguishable, whereas both differed significantly from the clinical group with psychotic symptoms (Group 2) for both points (p <0.01).

The emergence of “voices” may precede extensive psychotic experience, but it is rarely recorded before this stage due to varying perceptions of PLEs, their non-representation in clinical terms, and societal stigma leading to concealment. While the item concerning the presence of “voices inside the head” may be observed in both clinical and non-clinical groups, the item concerning “voices” is predominantly characteristic of the clinical group, indicating its severity as a measure of condition severity. Command AHs reflect negative content (“... can force me to do something I do not want”) and highlight a non-assertive relationship with the “voice”.

Understanding the alienation of thinking involves viewing “voices” as a dialogical manifestation of negative relationships and adverse social experiences in “extended” inner speech. PE, AH, and metacognitive assessments of experiences, including SA, are interconnected in this framework. Before individuals can identify episodes of mental distortions, anomalies in the structure of PE and PLE emerge, altering the perception of thinking, oneself, and the environment. These experiences become increasingly alienated and reattributed beyond the boundaries of the subject. The progression of PLE is directed toward increasing alienation, necessitating the study of phase transition mechanisms as a priority.

In the author’s model, the continuum of “voices” development signifies the reverse extension of internalized, automated mental activity. This progression ranges from the objectification of the thinking process and a lack of cognitive control to dissociative processes and a violation of self-perception, culminating in a violation of psyche agency.

C. Fernyhough [16] elaborated on an explanatory model of AH, focusing on dialogical thinking. Drawing from L.S. Vygotsky’s concept of inner speech as a product of ontogenetic development, which retains dialogical qualities from socially mediated interactions, Fernyhough delineates expanded and compressed forms of inner speech with varying syntactic and semantic configurations.

The four-phase model of speech internalization (including external speech, private speech, extended internal speech, and compressed internal speech) provides insights into the formation of AH:

1) The “violation of the internalization model” posits AH as a developmental deficit during the transition from expanded internal private speech to compressed internal speech.

2) The “re-extended model” suggests that compressed inner speech can re-extended during stressful situations, particularly those associated with social stress related to defeat. This model elucidates the negative content of “voices” and their corresponding communication manifestations.

When exploring the psychological characteristics of PLE and PE, significant direct correlations were found between the integrative indicator of PEQ and the frequency of PE experiences, along with metacognitive assessments of PE (r=0.432–0.602; p <0.0001) (Table 5).

 

Table 5. Scales of the Psychotic Experiences Questionnaire and metacognitive assessments of experience. social anxiety: Pearson correlation analysis

Таблица 5. Шкалы опросника психотических переживаний и метакогнитивные оценки опыта, социальной тревоги: корреляционный анализ Пирсона

Scales

Occur frequently

Accompanied by shame

Accompanied by guilt

Accompanied by anxiety

Difficult to control

Attempt to conceal ь

SA

1

Objectification of the thinking process

0.506***

0.347***

0.428***

0.562***

0.554***

0.433***

0.46***

2

Derealization. depersonalization

0.381***

0.397***

0.324***

0.363***

0.415***

0.420***

0.45***

Alienation of familiar experience

0.470***

0.425***

0.427***

0.479***

0.529***

0.459***

0.39***

Intrusive experiences “inside the head”

0.379***

0.422***

0.320***

0.487***

0.480***

0.423***

0.260*

3

Open-mindedness

0.175

0.187*

0.138

0.242**

0.274**

0.224*

0.318**

Experience of controllability from the outside

0.250**

0.392***

0.200*

0.197*

0.340***

0.324***

0.039

***р <0.0001; **р <0.001; *р <0.01–0.05.

 

Individual PEQ scales also have significant direct connections with metacognitive assessments, including emotional support for PE. Negative social emotions like shame and guilt, anxiety, difficulties in control, and attempts to conceal experiences, are associated with all parameters of the experience. However, only with the “openness of thoughts” scale, the correlation coefficients with the frequency of experience and the actualization of guilt do not reach the level of significance (p >0.05), although a direct relationship tendency existed.

The Pearson correlation analysis of SA and PEQ (Table 4) showed a significant influence of adverse social experiences and distress measures in assessment situations on the development of PE and PLE. Direct associations of SA were observed with all categories of PE, including “openness of thoughts”, except for the maximum level of alienation (“experience of controllability from the outside”). The absence of SA registration in cases of total violation of psyche agency through linear correlation analysis might stem from the challenge of correlating questionnaire statements with experiences as “own”. However, mediator models could demonstrate the presence of an association. In the early stages of PE, the role of the social-anxious component is evident in destabilizing the constrained nature of the psyche, thereby increasing the violation of self-perception, as indicated in self-reports. Over time, this connection may become more indirect [18, 19].

The use of Kendall’s correlation analysis to examine the relationship between SA and individual items of the PEQ concerning “voices” and the objectification of thinking showed statistically significant direct correlations with the following statements: “At times, thoughts seem to stop in my head” (τ=0.315; p <0.0001), “I find it difficult to control the way I think” (τ=0.315; p <0.0001), and “At times there are so many thoughts in my head that it is difficult to concentrate” (τ=0.277; p <0.0001). Furthermore, statements about intrusive phenomena were significantly correlated with SA: “A voice is sometimes heard inside or near the head” (τ=0.193; p <0.01) and “Unusual images or scenes appear inside my head, as if imposed from outside” (τ=0.159; p <0.05).

Social experience acts as both the psychological content and mechanism for the condensing and reverse extending of internal speech, leading to the transformation of internal dialogue into an external extended process. An important finding when analyzing AH in this context was the frequent negative content of AH or negative properties associated with the “voice” itself, which were linked to SA and sensitivity to interpersonal assessments. The connections observed between PE and SA provide evidence supporting the significance of social motives in an individual’s experiences.

CONCLUSION

This article highlights the modern theoretical concepts surrounding PE and PLE, including delirium and AH. Based on these concepts, the PEQ was developed, tested on both clinical and non-clinical samples, and subsequently validated. The theoretical configuration of the PE components underwent statistical verification. The structure of the questionnaire, identified through cluster analysis, was further validated through confirmatory factor analysis, demonstrating optimal consistency between the theoretical and empirical models.

The PEQ comprises three scales, conceptualized as stages of PE development or phase states, with transitions determined by systemic restructuring leading to escalating violations of self-perception and agency influenced by psychological patterns. These scales include subscales (six categories) that characterize the internal structure of each phase of PLE and PE:

Scale 1: “Objectification of thinking and control deficit” (with one subscale, “objectification of the thinking process”).

Scale 2: “Impaired self-perception and intrusive phenomena” (comprising three subscales: “derealization and depersonalization”, “alienation of habitual experience”, “intrusive experiences “inside the head” including voices, sounds, and scenes”).

Scale 3: “Violation of mental agency” (including two subscales: “openness of thoughts”, “experience of controllability from the outside”).

Cronbach’s alpha coefficient confirmed the high reliability and consistency of the questionnaire items. Convergent validity was established through Pearson correlation analysis, linking the PEQ scales with the SCL scales of “psychoticism”, “symptoms of paranoia”, and “obsessive-compulsive symptoms”, revealing significant statistical relationships.

Correlations of questionnaire items assessing the experience of “voices” validated the developed diagnostic tool’s adequacy in assessing the presence of AH. This assessment was observed at two stages: intrusions indicating a violation of self-perception (voices inside the head or nearby”) and a complete loss of agency (command voices).

The study also highlighted the enhancing role of shame, anxiety, SA, and guilt as metacognitive assessments in perpetuating PE. Shame, specifically retained a highly significant direct relationship with PE, even during the stage of maximum alienation. It acted as a negative social emotion, indicative of social defeat and self-stigmatization. Understanding shame’s reinforcing role in PE during phase transitions toward increased alienation was discussed in scientific contexts. SA was interconnected with all aspects of psychotic experience, influencing the objectification of thinking, actualization of voices, and complete inversion of agency. These relationships were statistically explained through the reinforcing influence of metacognitive strategies [18].

The subjective experience of uncontrollable obsessive intrusions into consciousness, vivid in their sensory-perceptual intensity, leads to a strong desire to regain control and stop the flow of unpleasant elements that interfere with the usual adaptation of thought content and mental imagery. Dissociative processes are intensified by SA and metacognitive assessments of experience, acting as mediators in the association between the objectification of thinking (the recurrent unfolding of inner speech under stress), which includes dialogical imagery and social situations, and the tendency to experience AH and PE.

PEQ serves as a valuable tool for screening studies on PLE and PE. It helps assess the severity of their overall presentation in a clinical context and individual scales that reflect the phases of extending PEs. Furthermore, it aids in determining the metacognitive assessment of these experiences, providing valuable information for constructing psychological intervention strategies that target the psychological variables supporting PE. This aspect will be discussed in more detail in the second part of this article.

RESTRICTIONS

The diagnosability of AH, despite the study’s anonymity, remains challenging. This challenge does not necessarily imply a low prevalence of such experiences. Instead, it reflects the resistance to acknowledging AH or the tendency to stigmatize/self-stigmatize PE, thereby reducing the probability of agreeing with questionnaire statements.

A part of the sample experiencing PE may interpret it differently (e.g., without labeling extended dialogues, voiced thoughts, or phrases from past conversations as “voices” or “hallucinations”). Some individuals may not currently experience PE or may not verbalize or share their experiences. Additionally, the scientific community lacks a consistent theory or model of PE that can integrate descriptive and experimental findings, explaining the systemic dynamic restructuring with increasing psychological alienation.

In clinical practice, maintaining confidentiality and ensuring optimal compliance are crucial when using the questionnaire. Negative attitudes toward the examination may distort the data due to symptom denial. In some cases, a response like “probably false” might indicate the presence of PE, but the impact of stigmatization/self-stigmatization may hinder agreeing with the experience.

 

1 SCL — Symptom Check List.

×

About the authors

Olga A. Sagalakova

Moscow State Psychological and Pedagogical University

Email: olgasagalakova@mail.ru
ORCID iD: 0000-0001-9975-1952
SPIN-code: 4455-7179

PhD, Cand. Sci. (Psichol.), Assoc. Prof., Senior Researcher, Laboratory of Experimental Pathopsychology

Russian Federation, Moscow

Dmitry V. Truevtsev

Moscow State Psychological and Pedagogical University

Author for correspondence.
Email: truevtsev@gmail.com
ORCID iD: 0000-0003-4246-2759
SPIN-code: 2983-0984

PhD, Cand. Sci. (Psichol.), Assoc. Prof., Senior Researcher

Russian Federation, Moscow

Olga V. Zhirnova

Moscow State Psychological and Pedagogical University

Email: olga.zhirnova.2015@mail.ru
ORCID iD: 0000-0002-6680-8286
SPIN-code: 6870-8526

Junior Researcher, Medical Psychologist

Russian Federation, Moscow

References

  1. Mendelevich VD, Gatin FF, Hamitov RR et al. Psihoticheskie simptomy pri nepsihoticheskih rasstrojstvah: oshibki diagnostiki ili novaya real’nost’? Nevrologicheskij vestnik. 2022;54(2):5–12. doi: 10.17816/nb108655. (In Russ.)
  2. Baryshnikov I, Suvisaari J, Aaltonen K et al. Self-reported psychosis-like experiences in patients with mood disorders. European Psychiatry. 2018;51:90–97. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2016.07.005
  3. Maijer K, Begemann MJH, Palmen SJMC et al. Auditory hallucinations across the lifespan: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine. 2018;48(6):879–888. doi: 10.1017/S0033291717002367
  4. Waters F, Collerton D, Ffytche DH et al. Visual hallucinations in the psychosis spectrum and comparative information from neurodegenerative disorders and eye disease. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 2014;40(4):233–245. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbu036
  5. Linscott RJ, van Os J. An updated and conservative systematic review and meta-analysis of epidemiological evidence on psychotic experiences in children and adults: on the pathway from proneness to persistence to dimensional expression across mental disorders. Psychological Medicine. 2013;43(6):1133–1149. doi: 10.1017/s0033291712001626
  6. Heilskov SER, Urfer-Parnas A, Nordgaard J. Delusions in the general population: A systematic review with emphasis on methodology. Schizophrenia Research. 2020;216:48–55. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2019.10.043
  7. Rubinshtejn SYa. Eksperimental’noe issledovanie obmanov sluha. In: Patopsihologiya. Hrestomatiya. Sost NL Belopol’skaya. M.: URAO; 1998. p. 59–72. (In Russ.)
  8. Bourgin J, Tebeka S, Mallet J et al. Prevalence and correlates of psychotic-like experiences in the general population. Schizophrenia Research. 2020;215:371–377. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2019.08.024
  9. Kusztrits I, Larøi F, Laloyaux J et al. Mapping psychotic-like experiences: Results from an online survey. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology. 2021;62(2):237–248. doi: 10.1111/sjop.12683
  10. Grattan RE, Mehta A, Clifford A. Disparities in psychosis risk symptoms for New Zealand Māori may be explained by systemic stressors and inappropriate conceptualization of culturally normative experiences. Schizophrenia Bulletin. 2023:156:sbad085. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbad085
  11. Karcher NR. Psychotic-like experiences in childhood and early adolescence: Clarifying the construct and future directions. Schizophrenia Research. 2022;246:205–206. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2022.06.030
  12. Van Os J, Reininghaus U. Psychosis as a transdiagnostic and extended phenotype in the general population. World Psychiatry. 2016;15(2):118–124. doi: 10.1002/wps.20310
  13. Scott KM, Saha S, Lim CW et al. Psychotic experiences and general medical conditions: A cross-national analysis based on 28 002 respondents from 16 countries in the WHO World Mental Health Surveys. Psychological Medicine. 2018;48(16):2730–2739. doi: 10.1017/S0033291718000363
  14. Khaled SM, Wilkins SS, Woodruff P. Lifetime prevalence and potential determinants of psychotic experiences in the general population of Qatar. Psychological Medicine. 2020;50(7):1110–1120. doi: 10.1017/S0033291719000977
  15. Kuznecov ON, Lebedev VI. Psihologiya i psihopatologiya odinochestva. M.: Kniga po trebovaniyu; 2013. 336 p. (In Russ.)
  16. Borghi AM, Fernyhough C. Concepts, abstractness and inner speech. Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences. 2023;378(1870):20210371. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2021.0371
  17. Longden E, Corstens D, Bowe S et al. A psychological intervention for engaging dialogically with auditory hallucinations (Talking With Voices): A single-site, randomised controlled feasibility trial. Schizophrenia Research. 2022;250:172–179. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2022.11.007
  18. Sagalakova OA, Truevcev DV, Zhirnova OV. Golosa v strukture psihoticheskih perezhivanij: rol' social'noj trevogi i metakognitivnyh strategij. Konsul'tativnaya psihologiya i psihoterapiya. 2023;31(4):86–104. doi: 10.17759/ cpp.2023310405. (In Russ.)
  19. Sagalakova OA, Truevcev DV, Zhirnova OV, Tinekov AV. Effekt social'noj trevogi i styda v razvitii golosov: metakognitivnaya obrabotka neblagopriyatnogo opyta otnoshenij i bditel'nost' k social'nym ugrozam. Klinicheskaya i special'naya psihologiya. 2023;12(2):25–53. doi: 10.17759/cpse.2023120202. (In Russ.)
  20. Ford JM. Studying auditory verbal hallucinations using the RDoC framework. Psychophysiology. 2016;53(3):298–304. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12457
  21. Megrabyan AA. Depersonalizaciya. Erevan: Armyanskoe gosudarstvennoe izdatel’stvo; 1962. 356 p. (In Russ.)
  22. Sheffield JM, Smith R, Suthaharan P et al. Relationships between cognitive biases, decision-making, and delusions. Scientific Reports. 2023;13(1):9485. doi: 10.1038/s41598-023-36526-1
  23. Klimusheva TA. Kliniko-psihologicheskie issledovaniya bol’nyh paranoidnoj formoj shizofrenii s sindromom Kandinskogo–Klerambo. In: Voprosy eksperimental’no patopsihologii. Pod red BV Zejgarnik, SYa Rubinshtejn. M.: NII psihiatrii; 1965. р. 117–128. (In Russ.)
  24. Sagalakova OA, Truevcev DV. Patopsixologiya social`noj trevogi i social`nogo trevozhnogo rasstrojstva. Tomsk: izd-vo Tomskogo universiteta; 2016. 140 p. (In Russ.)
  25. Dorahy M, Green MJ. Cognitive perspectives on dissociation and psychosis: Differences in the processing of threat. A Moskowitz, MJ Dorahy, I Schäfer eds. In: Psychosis, trauma and dissociation: Evolving perspectives on severe psychopathology. Wiley Blackwell; 2019. p. 283–303. doi: 10.1002/9781118585948.ch18
  26. Nesbit A, Dorahy MJ, Palmer R et al. Dissociation as a mediator between childhood abuse and hallucinations: An exploratory investigation using dissociative identity disorder and Schizophrenia Spectrum Disorders. Journal of Trauma and Dissociation. 2022;23(5):521–538. doi: 10.1080/15299732.2022.2064579
  27. Hayward M, Denney J, Vaughan S, Fowler D. The voice and you: Development and psychometric evaluation of a measure of relationships with voices. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy. 2008;15(1):45–52. doi: 10.1002/cpp.561
  28. Strauss C, Hugdahl K, Waters F et al. The Beliefs about Voices Questionnaire — Revised: A factor structure from 450 participants. Psychiatry Research. 2018;259:95–103. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2017.09.089
  29. McKay R, Langdon R, Coltheart M. “Sleights of mind”: Delusions, defences, and self-deception. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry. 2005;10(4):305–326. doi: 10.1080/13546800444000074
  30. Tarabrina NV. Psihologiya posttravmaticheskogo stressa. Blanki metodik. M.: Kogito-Centr; 2007. 77 p. (In Russ.)
  31. Stoet G. PsyToolkit: A novel web-based method for running online questionnaires and reaction-time experiments. Teaching of Psychology. 2017;44(1):24–31. doi: 10.1177/0098628316677643

Supplementary files

Supplementary Files
Action
1. JATS XML

Copyright (c) 2024 Eco-Vector

Creative Commons License
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.

СМИ зарегистрировано Федеральной службой по надзору в сфере связи, информационных технологий и массовых коммуникаций (Роскомнадзор).
Регистрационный номер и дата принятия решения о регистрации СМИ: серия ПИ № ФС 77 - 75562 от 12 апреля 2019 года.


This website uses cookies

You consent to our cookies if you continue to use our website.

About Cookies